IMR Press / JIN / Volume 22 / Issue 4 / DOI: 10.31083/j.jin2204089
Open Access Original Research
The Effect of Equine-Assisted Activities in Children Aged 7–8 Years Inhibitory Control: An fNIRS Study
Show Less
1 School of Kinesiology and Health, Capital University of Physical Education and Sports, 100191 Beijing, China
2 School of Sciences, Xi'an Technological University, 710021 Xi'an, Shaanxi, China
3 College of Public Education, Huainan Union University, 232038 Huainan, Anhui, China
*Correspondence: ahtangqian@126.com (Qian Tang); wuhao@cupes.edu.cn (Hao Wu)
These authors contributed equally.
J. Integr. Neurosci. 2023, 22(4), 89; https://doi.org/10.31083/j.jin2204089
Submitted: 27 February 2023 | Revised: 27 March 2023 | Accepted: 29 March 2023 | Published: 5 July 2023
Copyright: © 2023 The Author(s). Published by IMR Press.
This is an open access article under the CC BY 4.0 license.
Abstract

Background: Inhibitory control (IC), an important component of executive function, plays an important role in the overall development of children and has not been better studied in the field of equine-assisted activity (EAA). Therefore, this study investigated the effects of EAA on IC and the underlying brain neural mechanisms in children aged 7–8 years. Methods: Forty-eight healthy children aged 7–8 years from the Maple Leaf International School-Xi’an were randomly allocated to the equine-assisted activities group (EAAG) and control group (CG). The EAAG received 12 weeks of EAAG training from instructors at the MingLiu Horse Club while the CG continued their normal daily activities. The Flanker task was administered to both groups to assess IC pre- and post-intervention. Functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) data were collected during the Flanker task to examine the underlying neural mechanisms. Results: Our findings indicate that after 12 weeks of EAA, the EAAG performed significantly better on the Flanker tasks than the CG, with congruent and incongruent higher accuracy and faster reaction (p < 0.01). Importantly, fNIRS data analysis revealed increased oxyhemoglobin levels in the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (R-DLPFC) (p < 0.05) of the EAAG during the Flanker congruent task after the EAA intervention. Conclusions: Collectively, EAA demonstrated a positive impact on IC and could effectively activate R-DLPFC in children aged 7–8 years. Furthermore, it enhanced the activation of the brain regions related to IC and increased cognitive ability in children aged 7–8 years.

Keywords
equine-assisted activities
children
inhibitory control
flanker task
fNIRS
1. Introduction

Executive function, also known as executive control or cognitive control, is a critical top-down mental process that demands attention and attentive participation [1, 2, 3]. A recent study showed that executive function is an important predictor of a child’s physical and mental health, quality of life, performance at school, marital satisfaction and public safety [4]. Thus, it significantly impacts children’s development during the critical periods of growth and development [5].

Executive function comprises several interrelated cognitive processes, including inhibitory control (IC), working memory and cognitive flexibility [6, 7]. IC is a core component of executive function and is significantly associated with mental health, playing a pivotal role in all cognitive processes [8]. IC refers to an individual’s ability to consciously control, inhibit, or override a superior response or ignore irrelevant information or environmental distractions by focusing instead on relevant information [7]. Several task paradigms have been used to study IC [6], including the Flanker task, Go/No-Go task, Stroop task, Simon task and Stop signal task. IC can be divided into two subcategories: controlled attention and cognitive inhibition [9]. Moreover, IC plays a critical role in solving complex problems such as mathematical questions [10], learning challenges [11] and emotional control [12]. Therefore, it is imperative to identify the factors that enhance IC [13].

Previous research showed that IC could improve performance in multiple sports. Studies have investigated the use of sports equipment in increasing IC. For instance, Spitzer et al. [14] divided 24 students into a TV-watching group and a basketball group and performed the Flanker task before and after 30 minutes of playing basketball and watching TV, respectively. The results showed that the basketball group demonstrated superior IC than the TV-watching group [14]. Similarly, Wen et al. [15] randomly assigned 145 children aged 7–8 years to a 40-minute resistance training, coordination training, soccer training and control group and conducted Go/No-Go task tests before and after the experiment. They reported that the soccer group showed faster responses and higher accuracy [15]. Additionally, physical exercise has also been shown to enhance IC. Cho et al. [16] randomly assigned 30 healthy elementary school students to the control and Taekwondo groups and used the Stroop task to test the participants’ IC, with the latter receiving 60 minutes of Taekwondo training 5 times a week for 16 weeks. After the intervention, the Stroop task test scores were significantly higher in the Taekwondo group [16]. Despite the promising findings of previous research on the impact of sports equipment training or exercising on IC, there has been limited investigation into the effects of equine-assisted activities (EAA) on IC.

EAA represents a specific subgroup of activities of animal-assisted interventions comprising therapeutic horseback riding, vaulting, carriage driving and other non-riding activities with animals [17, 18]. It uses various methods to guide interactions between humans and horses and promote positive activities that improve human physical functions and emotional well-being, which can alleviate physical and mental problems and improve human health and happiness [19]. Although EAA has demonstrated improvements in executive function [20], the underlying cerebral neural mechanisms via which EAA interventions improve individual cognitive function are poorly understood. To address this gap, functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) was used to assess changes in the concentration of oxygenated and oxyhemoglobin molecules in the blood, providing a non-invasive method to measure cerebral blood oxygenation mechanisms in response to EAA [21]. Despite the potential of fNIRS in measuring the cerebral blood oxygen mechanism in EAA [22], few studies have used it to measure the cognitive benefits of EAA in the prefrontal and motor cortex of children aged 7–8 years.

The study aimed to investigate the impact of a 12-week EAA intervention on IC in 7–8-year-old children using fNIRS to explore neurological mechanisms in the prefrontal and motor cortex by measuring cerebral blood oxygenation during a cognitive task to provide insights into the impact of EAA on IC.

2. Methods
2.1 Subjects and Study Design
2.1.1 Subjects

The G*Power software (version 3.1.9.7; Franz Faul, University Kiel, Germany) was used to estimate the study sample size [23]. The specific parameters were set as follows: α = 0.05, power = 0.85, effect size = 0.35, statistical test = repeated measures, number of groups = 2, and number of measurements = 2. Under these conditions, the resulting sample size was 11 subjects per group. However, 50 subjects were recruited to improve the reliability of the study results. However, since two participants did not complete the 12-week EAA intervention, the overall cohort comprised 48 participants. They were recruited from Maple Leaf International School-Xi’an in a 1:1 boys and girls ratio. They were randomly assigned to an equine-assisted activity group (EAAG) and a control group (CG). They were instructed and recommended to avoid colds, fevers and sports injuries and continued to participate in training for 12 weeks, while the CG did not engage in more intense physical activity. The Institutional Ethical Committee of the Capital University of Physical Education and Sports (Beijing, China) approved all procedures and protocols (No.2021A41). It should be noted that all subjects were accompanied by their parents as they learned about the contents of the training intervention and signed an informed consent form. The specific information of all participants is shown in Table 1.

Table 1.Basic information.
Group Number Age Height (cm) Weight (kg) BMI (kg/m2)
EAAG 24 7–8 126.6 ± 6.1 26.7 ± 3.6 16.6 ± 1.4
CG 24 7–8 128.6 ± 8.2 26.5 ± 3.5 16.0 ± 1.6

BMI, body mass index; EAAG, equine-assisted activities group; CG, control group.

Previous studies showed that medications [24], obesity [25], intelligence [26] and activity frequency [27] may impact IC. However, different from other sports, EAA has certain requirements for subjects. Specific requirements are shown in Table 2.

Table 2.Specific requirements information.
Inclusion criteria Exclusion criteria
(1) Age 7–8 years with normal intelligence and no cognitive disorders (1) Obesity, BMI 24
(2) Right-handed (2) Motor impairment and physical disability
(3) In good health, without sports injuries and mental illnesses and taking no medications (3) Participation in multiple sports training on Saturdays and Sundays
(4) Not enrolled in horseback riding training in the last 6 months (4) Unwillingness to cooperate with horseback riding-related movements in the experimental intervention
(5) No history of horsehair allergies (5) Fear of horses
(6) No fear of horses, with boldness, strong will and high interest in horseback riding
2.1.2 Study Design

A randomized controlled experimental design of 2 (group: EAAG, CG) × 2 (time: pre-test, post-test) was used in this study. The subjects were numbered and randomly divided into 2 groups of 24 each by the randomized function method in Excel 2019 (Microsoft., Redmond, WA, USA). The relevant experimental pre-test indicators included demographic variables (height and weight), Flanker task (congruent and incongruent task response times and accuracy) and fNIRS data (prefrontal and motor cortex oxyhemoglobin) during the completion of the Flanker task. The post-test was conducted at the end of the 12-week EAA intervention, and the test indicators were consistent with the pre-test (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1.

Flow diagram of the study design. EAAG, equine-assisted activities group; CG, control group; NIRS, nearinfrared spectroscopy.

2.2 Intervention

The experimental protocol utilized in this study was adapted from Cook et al.’s work [28], entitled “Incorporating Game in Hippotherapy A Companion Book to the Brown Pony Series”, which has been shown effective in improving various aspects of cognitive function, social interaction skills, neural control and coordination in riders. The content of the 12 weeks of EAA training is shown in Table 3.

Table 3.Equine-assisted activity (EAA) training Periodic protocol.
Time Content
Weeks 1–4 (Basic training week)
Week 1 Knowledge about horses and the enhancement of children’s sensory processing abilities.
Week 2 Sensory processing abilities, the ability to perceive things and increase self-confidence and memory.
Week 3 Brief rides under the guidance of an equestrian instructor to enhance self-affirmation and further improve sensory processing.
Week 4 Establishment of correct neuromuscular control, training of children’s cognitive functions.
Weeks 5–8 (Improvement training week)
Week 5 Perceptual processing abilities, proprioception and spatial awareness.
Week 6 Perceptual processing abilities, proprioceptive and spatial senses, and core control.
Week 7 Perceptual processing abilities, proprioceptive and spatial, increase the gait of the horse for neuromuscular control in children.
Week 8 Perceptual processing abilities, proprioceptive and spatial, enhance neuromuscular control of the horse’s gait for the children through rhythmic changes.
Weeks 9–12 (Intensive training week)
Week 9 Perceptual processing abilities, proprioceptive and spatial, enhance neuromuscular control of the horse’s gait for the children through rhythmic changes.
Week 10 Perceptual processing abilities, proprioceptive and spatial, enhancing neuromuscular control of the horse’s gait for children through rhythmic changes.
Week 11 Perceptual processing abilities, proprioceptive and spatial; enhancement of neuromuscular control of the horse’s gait for children through rhythmic changes; enhancement of children’s memory and attention.
Week 12 Perceptual processing ability, proprioception, and spatial sense; enhancement of neuromuscular control of children’s gait through rhythmic changes; enhancement of children’s attention, discrimination, and information processing ability.

The 12-week EAA comprised 2 training sessions per week for 45–55 minutes each. The experimental intervention took place every week 1 and 3 from 15:50–17:50 and was performed by 6 MingLiu Horse Club instructors, each responsible for 2–3 children. An example of the lesson is shown in Fig. 2 [28].

Fig. 2.

Flow diagram of an EAA Intervention lesson.

2.3 Measures

Each EAAG and CG subject underwent a pre-test within 1 week prior to training and a post-test within 3 days after training.

2.3.1 Flanker Task

The Flanker task, a traditional conflict paradigm for studying the influence of task-irrelevant information on processing task-relevant information [29], was used for assessing IC [6]. It was designed using the E-Prime software (version 2.0, Psychology Software Tools Inc., Pittsburgh, PA, USA). The congruent and incongruent Accuracy and Reaction times (RT) statistics were performed on the test results. During the experimental task, the participants were instructed to focus on the “+” symbol in the center of the screen as a cue to start the task. Subsequently, a sequence of five letter combinations was displayed on the screen for 1000 ms, with the middle arrow serving as the point of gaze and a stimulus interval of 1 second. This sequence of letters can occur in the following two conditions: consistent conditions, such as “FFFFFF” and “LLLLLL”, and inconsistent conditions, such as “LLFLL” and “FFLLFF”. The participants were required to respond as quickly and correctly as possible to the middle letter of each sequence by pressing the “F” key on the keyboard with their index finger if it was an “F” and the “L” key if it was an “L”. The two conditions were presented in an equal and randomized manner, with the formal test comprising two segments, each of which required 60 judgments and 12 practice sessions before the formal test.

2.3.2 fNIRS Measurements

In this study, a portable NirSmart63 was used to collect raw signal light intensities from the prefrontal and motor cortex of the brain during the Flanker task. Then, we calculated the mean value concentration change of oxyhemoglobin (Oxy-HB) in the prefrontal and motor cortex using the absorbance difference based on the improved Beer-Lambert law [30]. The experimental acquisition system comprised 14 signal sources and 14 detectors, forming the following 35 channels: 1=S1-D1, 2=S1-D6, 3=S2-D2, 4=S2-D7, 5=S3-D2, 6=S3-D3, 7=S3-D8, 8=S4-D3, 9=S4-D4, 10=S4-D9, 11=S5-D4, 12=S5-D10, 13=S6-D5, 14=S6-D11, 15=S7-D1, 16=S7-D6, 17=S7-D12, 18=S7-D13, 19=S8-D2, 20=S8-D7, 21=S8-D8, 22=S9-D3, 23=S9-D8, 24=S9-D9, 25=S10-D4, 26 =S10-D9, 27=S10-D10, 28=S11-D5, 29=S11-D11, 30=S12-D12, 31=S12-D13, 32=S13-D11, 33=S13-D14, 34=S14-D11 and 35=S14-D14. According to Bordmann’s partition, they were placed in the prefrontal and motor cortex, respectively (Fig. 3: Top, Front, Right and Left). The distance between the two probes on the signal acquisition cap was 3 cm, and they were arranged in a certain pattern to monitor the signals of the 35 channels covering the prefrontal and motor cortex. In addition, the more pronounced the activation of brain areas, the darker the color on the brain area map (pink > purple > blue).

Fig. 3.

Distribution of signal sources and detectors in the prefrontal and motor cortex.

2.4 Statistics and Analysis

Descriptive results are reported as means ± standard deviations. The assumption of normality was verified using the Shapiro-Wilk test. The t-test was used to perform the difference test between the EAAG and CG and the Oxy-Hb of each channel. The Flanker task data were analyzed using repeated measures Analysis of Variance (ANOVA). All statistical analyses were performed using the SPSS software version 26.0 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA), and significant differences are indicated by p < 0.05, whereby * represents p < 0.05 and ** represents p < 0.01.

3. Results
3.1 Demographic Variables and Flanker Task Difference Examination

The demographic variables included age, height, weight, body mass index (BMI), and the Flanker task, which included congruent and incongruent Accuracy and RT. The data were assessed for the EAAG and CG samples to reduce the effect of these factors on the experimental results by excluding demographic and IC differences between the two groups (Table 4).

Table 4.Demographic and Flanker task data difference comparison.
Variables EAAG CG T p
Years 7.5 ± 0.5 7.5 ± 0.5 - -
Height (cm) 126.6 ± 6.1 128.6 ± 8.2 –0.94 0.35
Weight (kg) 26.7 ± 3.6 26.5 ± 3.5 0.45 0.96
BMI (kg/m2) 16.6 ± 1.4 16.0 ± 1.6 0.23 0.82
Accuracy (%) Congruent task 0.86 ± 0.06 0.86 ± 0.06 –0.22 0.83
Incongruent task 0.84 ± 0.07 0.86 ± 0.07 –1.12 0.25
RT (ms) Congruent task 718.1 ± 100.3 708.6 ± 81.6 0.36 0.72
Incongruent task 771.5 ± 87.9 762.9 ± 90.7 0.34 0.74

RT, reaction times.

Before conducting the independent samples t-test and chi-square test, all data underwent normal distribution testing, with a significance level of p > 0.05. We observed no significant difference between the EAAG and CG groups in terms of height (cm) (126.6 ± 6.1 vs. 128.6 ± 8.2; T = –0.94, p = 0.35), weight (kg) (26.7 ± 3.6 vs. 26.5 ± 3.5; T = 0.45, p = 0.96), BMI (kg/m2) (16.6 ± 1.4 vs. 16.0 ± 1.6; T = 0.23, p = 0.82), congruent task accuracy (0.86 ± 0.06 vs. 0.86 ± 0.06; T = –0.22, p = 0.83), incongruent task accuracy (0.84 ± 0.07 vs. 0.86 ± 0.07; T = –1.12, p = 0.25), congruent task RT (718.1 ± 100.3 vs. 708.6 ± 81.6; T = 0.36, p = 0.72) and incongruent task RT (771.5 ± 87.9 vs. 762.9 ± 90.7; T = 0.34, p = 0.74). Altogether, these results indicated no difference in demographic variables and Flanker task between the two groups in the pre-test.

3.2 Flanker Task Results

To investigate the changes in EAA on the IC of children aged 7–8 years, this study used 2 (group: EAAG, CG) × 2 (time: pre-test, post-test) repeated measures ANOVA to determine the Flanker task data pre-and-post and the results (Table 5).

Table 5.Statistical results of repeated measures ANOVA for two groups.
Variables df F p η2 partial
Congruent task accuracy (%)
Time 1 12.91 0.001** 0.22
Time*Group 1 9.21 0.004** 0.17
Error 46
Group 1 0.58 0.45 0.01
Error 46
Incongruent task accuracy (%)
Time 1 5.85 0.02* 0.11
Time*Group 1 3.29 0.08 0.07
Error 46
Group 1 0.46 0.51 0.10
Error 46
Congruent task RT (ms)
Time 1 11.52 0.001** 0.20
Time*Group 1 19.42 0.000** 0.30
Error 46
Group 1 0.87 0.36 0.20
Error 46
Incongruent task RT (ms)
Time 1 40.57 0.000** 0.47
Time*Group 1 24.49 0.000** 0.35
Error 46
Group 1 0.89 0.35 0.02
Error 46

Note: *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01.

In the congruent task accuracy (%), no significant main effect of group was observed, F(1,46) = 0.58, p = 0.45 > 0.05, η2 partial = 0.01, while there was a significant main effect of time, F(1,46) = 12.91, p = 0.001 < 0.01, η2 partial = 0.22, indicating a significant interaction effect of group*time, F(1,46) = 9.21, p = 0.004 < 0.01, η2 partial = 0.17 (Table 5). In the analysis of simple effects, a highly significant difference (p < 0.01) pre-and-post experiment was observed in EAAG, with a congruent task accuracy (%) pre-and-post experiment of 0.85 ± 0.06 and 0.89 ± 0.05, indicating an increase by 0.04, as well as in CG, which was 0.84 ± 0.07 and 0.87 ± 0.05, with an increase of 0.03 (Table 6).

Table 6.Comparison of Flanker task data differences between the two groups.
Test name Group Pre-test Post-test WGV
Mean of values
Congruent task accuracy (%) EAAG 0.85 ± 0.06 0.89 ± 0.05 0.04**
CG 0.84 ± 0.07 0.87 ± 0.05 0.03
DBG 0.03 0.02
Incongruent task accuracy (%) EAAG 0.84 ± 0.07 0.87 ± 0.05 0.03**
CG 0.86 ± 0.07 0.87 ± 0.06 0.01
DBG –0.02 0.01
Congruent task RT (ms) EAAG 718.07 ± 100.34 663.52 ± 71.65 –54.55**
CG 708.89 ± 90.61 715.63 ± 74.57 6.74
DBG 9.18 –52.11**
Incongruent task RT (ms) EAAG 771.07 ± 87.88 699.86 ± 73.69 –71.21**
CG 762.89 ± 90.68 753.91 ± 92.94 –8.98
DBG 8.18 –54.05*

Note: WGV, Within-group variation; DBG, Differences between groups; *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01.

In the incongruent task accuracy (%), there was no significant main effect of group, F(1,46) = 0.46, p = 0.51 > 0.05, η2 partial = 0.10, while there was a significant main effect of time, F(1,46) = 5.85, p = 0.02 < 0.05, η2 partial = 0.11, as shown by post hoc multiple comparisons, whereby the EAAG of pre-and-post experiment measurements were highly significantly different (p < 0.01), while no significant interaction effect for group*time was observed, F(1,46) = 3.29, p = 0.08 > 0.05, η2 partial = 0.07 (Table 5).

In the congruent task RT (ms), there was no significant main effect of group, F(1,46) = 0.87, p = 0.36 > 0.05, η2 partial = 0.20, but there was a significant main effect of time, F(1,46) = 11.52, p = 0.001 < 0.01, η2 partial = 0.20. In addition, there was also a significant interaction effect of group*time in the congruent task RT, F(1,46) = 19.42, p = 0.000 < 0.01, η2 partial = 0.30 (Table 5). In the analysis by simple effects, a highly significant difference was observed in EAAG pre-and-post experiment (p < 0.01), with a congruent task RT of 718.07 ± 100.34 and 663.52 ± 71.65 (an improvement of 54.55), and in CG, with a congruent task RT pre-and-post experiment of 708.89 ± 90.61 and 715.63 ± 74.57 (a decrease of 6.55). The congruent task RT in the EAAG improved by 61.29 compared to the CG (Table 6). However, in terms of targeting between groups, there was a highly significant difference between the EAAG and CG post-experiment (p < 0.01).

In the incongruent task RT (ms), there was no significant main effect of group, F(1,46) = 0.89, p = 0.35 > 0.05, η2 partial = 0.02, while there was a significant main effect of time, F(1,46) = 40.57, p = 0.000 < 0.01, η2 partial = 0.47. Additionally, we also observed a significant interaction of group*time effect, F(1,46) = 24.49, p = 0.000 < 0.01, η2 partial = 0.35 (Table 5). In the analysis by simple effects, there was a highly significant difference in EAAG pre-and-post experiment (p < 0.01), with an incongruent task RT of 771.07 ± 87.88 and 699.86 ± 73.69 (an improvement of 71.21), and in the CG, with an incongruent task RT pre-and-post experiment of 762.89 ± 90.68 and 753.91 ± 92.94 (an improvement of 8.21). The incongruent task RT of the EAAG was 62.23 higher than that of the CG (Table 6).

3.3 fNIRS Results

After 12 weeks of EAA intervention, the raw signals from the congruent task test were collected using NirSmart63 during the Flanker task in the two groups, followed by transformation using the NirSpark software (version 1.7.5, NirScan, HuiChuang, Beijing, China) to derive the change in Oxy-Hb concentration in each channel in the prefrontal and motor cortex in the EAAG and CG, respectively. The mean values of each channel in each of the 24 subjects were processed separately and combined with the paired-samples t-test to yield significant differences in the changes of each channel pre-and-post EAAG and CG, respectively (Table 7).

Table 7.Channel changes of Oxy-Hb in each channel.
Channel S-D EAAG T p CG T p
Pre Post Pre Post
1 S1-D1 –0.0035 0.0042 –1.34 0.27 –0.0178 –0.0076 0.24 0.83
2 S1-D6 0.0078 –0.0040 0.78 0.49 0.0067 0.0025 1.65 0.14
3 S2-D2 0.0012 0.0013 –0.08 0.94 –0.0155 –0.0165 –0.13 0.90
4 S2-D7 0.0007 –0.0001 0.49 0.65 –0.0300 –0.0228 –1.14 0.11
5 S3-D2 0.0041 0.0058 –0.39 0.71 0.0014 –0.0202 1.20 0.30
6 S3-D3 –0.0008 0.0028 –0.89 0.42 0.0023 –0.0180 1.82 0.14
7 S3-D8 0.0003 0.0055 –3.08 0.04* –0.0045 –0.0176 1.79 0.15
8 S4-D3 –0.0007 0.0052 –0.84 0.45 0.0043 –0.0090 1.28 0.27
9 S4-D4 0.0052 0.0071 –0.29 0.79 –0.0098 –0.0184 0.83 0.45
10 S4-D9 –0.0022 0.0038 –1.28 0.27 0.0063 0.0003 0.51 0.64
11 S5-D4 –0.0044 0.0043 –1.91 0.13 –0.0074 –0.0088 0.88 0.43
12 S5-D10 –0.0017 0.0022 –1.87 0.14 –0.0218 –0.0129 –0.81 0.46
13 S6-D5 0.0082 0.0026 0.94 0.42 0.0065 –0.0165 2.31 0.10
14 S6-D11 0.0111 0.0028 0.87 0.45 –0.0167 –0.0158 1.89 0.16
15 S7-D1 0.0043 0.0052 –0.41 0.71 –0.0155 0.0000 –0.84 0.46
16 S7-D6 0.0057 0.0020 –0.02 0.99 0.0003 –0.0136 –0.16 0.88
17 S7-D12 0.0097 0.0084 –0.04 0.97 –0.0277 –0.0285 –0.92 0.42
18 S7-D13 0.0011 0.0049 –0.77 0.50 0.0085 –0.0171 0.88 0.45
19 S8-D2 0.0036 0.0033 0.07 0.95 0.0097 –0.0117 1.22 0.29
20 S8-D7 0.0039 0.0001 0.67 0.54 0.0020 0.0014 –0.08 0.94
21 S8-D8 0.0044 0.0085 –4.28 0.01* 0.0081 –0.0149 0.75 0.49
22 S9-D3 –0.0004 0.0057 –2.05 0.11 –0.0094 –0.0121 0.03 0.98
23 S9-D8 –0.0040 0.0020 –4.09 0.01* 0.0085 –0.0192 0.70 0.52
24 S9-D9 –0.0042 –0.0002 –0.89 0.42 0.0236 –0.0291 2.06 0.11
25 S10-D4 –0.0022 0.0060 –1.33 0.25 –0.0097 –0.0016 0.31 0.77
26 S10-D9 0.0003 0.0037 –1.16 0.31 0.0065 –0.0140 1.60 0.18
27 S10-D10 0.0019 0.0062 –0.80 0.47 0.0198 –0.0121 2.41 0.07
28 S11-D5 –0.0026 –0.0046 0.28 0.79 0.0008 0.0175 –1.87 0.16
29 S11-D11 –0.0025 0.0017 –0.79 0.49 0.0246 –0.0051 0.65 0.56
30 S12-D12 0.0059 0.0006 0.80 0.48 –0.0198 –0.0179 –0.03 0.97
31 S12-D13 0.0134 0.0042 0.85 0.46 0.0000 –0.0045 –0.15 0.89
32 S13-D11 0.0114 0.0010 1.02 0.38 0.0055 0.0018 0.48 0.67
33 S13-D14 0.0037 0.0031 0.02 0.99 –0.0157 –0.0216 0.71 0.53
34 S14-D11 0.0004 0.0046 –1.00 0.39 0.0011 0.0001 –0.04 0.97
35 S14-D14 –0.0041 0.0041 –0.97 0.40 0.0062 –0.0107 1.01 0.39

Note: *p < 0.05.

In this study, a pair of adjacent light sources and a detector were used to form a channel, and we calculated the intra-group average of the mean at the channel level. Then, the image was generated by the interpolation method of inverse distance [31], as shown in Figs. 4,5.

Fig. 4.

Brain activation of prefrontal and motor cortex congruent task test in EAAG. The color reflects the mean of each region on the time scale, and the purple color represents higher activation than the blue–colored regions. The gray node represents the channel formed by each light source probe and detector probe, and the channel number is the label of the channel.

Fig. 5.

Brain activation of prefrontal and motor cortex congruent task test in CG.

The values of changes in Oxy-Hb in each channel in the prefrontal and motor cortex in the pre-and-post congruent task in EAAG and CG were examined by a paired-sample t-test. The results showed a statistical difference (p < 0.05) between the EAAG pre-and-post in channels 7, 21 and 23 (pink areas), as shown in Fig. 6. However, there was no statistical difference in CG. According to Bordmann, channels 7, 21 and 23 belong to the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (R-DLPFC).

Fig. 6.

Brain activation of significant channel regions in EAAG.

In the Flanker task test, the paired-sample t-test for the incongruent task individual channels data was not significantly different between EAAG and CG (p > 0.05).

4. Discussion

Our results showed that 12 weeks of EAA in EAAG can improve performance based on the Flanker task, as indicated by a reduction in RT and an increase in accuracy, and enhance brain activation of the R-DLPFC in children aged 7–8. Thus, EAA may improve the IC and enhance the activation of the brain regions related to IC, at least using this study setting.

4.1 IC Changes

Our findings suggest that 12 weeks of EAA increased IC to some extent in children aged 7–8 years. Previous studies have shown that IC development rates vary at different stages. Anderson et al. [32] found that 6–7 years was a sensitive period for development, with growth slowing down after age 7 and leveling off after age 10. In this study, the subjects belonged to this age group. Training load has always been an important aspect of sports and must be emphasized, as the amount of exercise intensity and time may also directly affects IC. A previous study showed that a chronic exercise cycle of 12–24 weeks, with an exercise time of 50–90 min/week and frequency of 2–3 times/week, significantly affected IC [33]. The training load requirements of our experiment were largely consistent with these findings and may have a positive impact. Additionally, supportive evidence was found in a study on open- and closed-skill sports [34]. It is well known that closed skills sports are performed in a relatively stable and predictable environment, in which motor actions are repetitive and unrelated to the external environment, whereas open skills sports are performed in a dynamic and changing environment that requires constant adaptation to external stimuli [35]. EAA is an open-skill sport that requires overcoming the distractions of the external environment, maintaining a high level of concentration, and ultimately automating a complex physical activity. These complex movements require increased cognitive involvement [36], avoiding incorrect technical movements and activating more brain systems [37]. Thus, the nervous system regulates and controls both simple and complex movements, with the brain playing the most significant and prominent regulatory role. Related studies have found that sports with greater cognitive involvement are more effective in exerting further positive effects on children’s cognitive abilities than sports without cognitive involvement [37]. In addition, riders are required to maintain a high degree of attention and tension during the whole process, which helps improve their attention retention. Studies have shown that EAA can effectively improve children’s attention [38, 39] and may further improve their IC.

4.2 Brain Activation Status

In this study, it was observed that 12-week EAA activated the R-DLPFC in the Flanker congruent task. The changes in the Oxy-Hb in the R-DLPFC of the brain indicate an increase in local cerebral blood flow triggered by the activation of neuronal cells in the cerebral cortex. According to the principle of neural cell coupling, increased local brain oxygenation suggests that the brain needs to recruit more neural resources [40], which in turn indicates that EAA training requires constant activation of neural processing associated with the R-DLPFC to adapt to the more complex cognitive demands of the relevant training task. It has been shown that high accuracy during the execution of attentional network tasks may originate from the high neural activation of the right frontoparietal network [41], which showed a clear right hemisphere dominance [42, 43]. Moreover, meta-analysis correlation results also showed that the brain activation sites associated with the Flanker task were mainly found in the R-DLPFC [44] and that different types of cue conditions were associated with frontoparietal network activation. Comparatively, R-DLPFC is thought to be an important component of neural processing in the right frontoparietal network [45]. Significant activation in this region also reflected reliable performance in extrinsic behavior, as indicated by the observed increased accuracy and decreased reaction time in the Flanker task. The dorsolateral prefrontal cortex coordinates the rest of the human brain and plays a crucial role in arithmetic and executive functions. Numerous investigations closely related to the activation model of brain regions in the Flanker task show that the activation is mainly present in the R-DLPFC [44]. In addition, fNIRS has also been used to monitor prefrontal cortex oxygenation in other sports and showed that testing different sports effectively activated R-DLPFC. Miao Yu et al. [46] found significant activation of the R-DLPFC in all three groups through a comparison between elite, expert and novice ice hockey athletes, which was concordant with our present study.

Neuropsychological and neuroimaging findings link dorsolateral prefrontal activation and top-down attentional control [47]. In addition, recent findings reveal an important role of R-DLPFC in task preparation [48], which plays an irreplaceable and important role in human development. In this present research, the children selected were 7–8 years old and did not undergo relevant training, and there was a greater need to enhance top-down attentional control to always pay attention to the outside world and changes in the trainer’s commands. This continuous state probably enhanced the attentional control of the riders, signifying that EAA training might lead to greater stimulation of the right R-DLPFC, which is related to attentional control in cognitive function. Previous studies have shown the importance of R-DLPFC activation in top-down attention [49]. Thus, it could be deduced that EAA training activated the R-DLPFC and improved the ability to process conflicting information, which in turn enhanced participants’ cognitive functioning. The result of better performance in completing cognitive tasks provides neurophysiological evidence that EAA training promotes the development of cognitive function and further supports the behavioral findings of this study.

Other prefrontal and motor cortex regions did not show significant activation changes. One possible explanation could be that cognitive changes depend to a substantial extent on prefrontal cortex function, which leads to a possible reason for the lack of significant changes before and after activation of the motor cortex. In addition, important subcortical areas, such as the hippocampus, are difficult to detect and could be considered a limitation of the present study.

5. Conclusions

The EAA positively impacts IC and effectively activates R-DLPFC. It also demonstrated promising abilities to activate the brain regions related to IC and increase cognitive ability in children aged 7–8 years.

Availability of Data and Materials

The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Author Contributions

HW and QT designed the research study. LQ and YZF performed data processing. XDC for data processing, interpretation of results, editing and writing. All authors contributed to editorial changes in the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript. All authors have participated sufficiently in the work and agreed to be accountable for all aspects of the work.

Ethics Approval and Consent to Participate

All participants signed a written informed consent form according to the Helsinki Declaration and satisfied the criteria for fNIRS. The Institutional Ethical Committee of the Capital University of Physical Education and Sports (Beijing, China) approved all procedures and protocols (No.2021A41).

Acknowledgment

Not applicable.

Funding

This project was supported by the key techniques of physical function characteristics of athletes in wheelchair curling, cross-country skiing and biathlon in Winter Paralympic Games (Grant no. PXM2020_014206_000016).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References
[1]
Burgess PW, Simons JS. Theories of frontal lobe executive function: clinical applications. Effectiveness of Rehabilitation for Cognitive Deficits. 2005; 2: 211–232.
[2]
Espy KA. Using developmental, cognitive, and neuroscience approaches to understand executive control in young children. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2004; 26: 379–384.
[3]
Miller EK, Cohen JD. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2001; 24: 167–202.
[4]
Gai X S, Xu J, Yan Y, Wang Y, Xie X. Exergame can improve children’s executive function: The role of physical intensity and cognitive engagement. Acta Psychologica Sinica. 2021; 53: 505–514.
[5]
McClelland MM, Cameron C. Developing together: The role of executive function and motor skills in children’s early academic lives. Early Childhood Research Quarterly. 2019; 46: 142–151.
[6]
Diamond A. Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology. 2013; 64: 135–168.
[7]
Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A, Wager TD. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “Frontal Lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology. 2000; 41: 49–100.
[8]
Smith EE, Jonides J. Storage and executive processes in the frontal lobes. Science. 1999; 283: 1657–1661.
[9]
Braver TS. The variable nature of cognitive control: a dual mechanisms framework. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2012; 16: 106–113.
[10]
Coulanges L, Abreu-Mendoza RA, Varma S, Uncapher MR, Gazzaley A, Anguera J, et al. Linking inhibitory control to math achievement via comparison of conflicting decimal numbers. Cognition. 2021; 214: 104767.
[11]
Lee HW, Lo YH, Li KH, Sung WS, Juan CH. The relationship between the development of response inhibition and intelligence in preschool children. Frontiers in Psychology. 2015; 6: 802.
[12]
Hudson A, Jacques S. Put on a happy face! Inhibitory control and socioemotional knowledge predict emotion regulation in 5- to 7-year-olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2014; 123: 36–52.
[13]
Zagaria T, Antonucci G, Buono S, Recupero M, Zoccolotti P. Executive Functions and Attention Processes in Adolescents and Young Adults with Intellectual Disability. Brain Sciences. 2021; 11: 42.
[14]
Spitzer MWH, Furtner M. Being physically active versus watching physical activity–Effects on inhibitory control. Trends in Neuroscience Education. 2016; 5: 30–33.
[15]
Wen X, Yang Y, Wang F. Influence of acute exercise on inhibitory control and working memory of children: a comparison between soccer, resistance, and coordinative exercises. International Journal of Sport Psychology. 2021; 52: 101–119.
[16]
Cho SY, So WY, Roh HT. The Effects of Taekwondo Training on Peripheral Neuroplasticity-Related Growth Factors, Cerebral Blood Flow Velocity, and Cognitive Functions in Healthy Children: A Randomized Controlled Trial. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2017; 14: 454.
[17]
Lanning BA, Baier MEM, Ivey-Hatz J, Krenek N, Tubbs JD. Effects of equine assisted activities on autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2014; 44: 1897–1907.
[18]
Ozyurt G, Özcan K, Dinsever Elikucuk Ç, Odek U, Akpinar S. Equine Assisted Activities Have Positive Effects on Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder and Family Functioning. Montenegrin Journal of Sports Science and Medicine. 2020; 9: 51–58.
[19]
Arrazola A, Merkies K. Effect of Human Attachment Style on Horse Behaviour and Physiology during Equine-Assisted Activities-A Pilot Study. Animals. 2020; 10: 1156.
[20]
Gilboa Y, Helmer A. Self-Management Intervention for Attention and Executive Functions Using Equine-Assisted Occupational Therapy Among Children Aged 6-14 Diagnosed with Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine. 2020; 26: 239–246.
[21]
Sonkaya AR. The Use of Functional Near Infrared Spectroscopy Technique in Neurology. NeuroQuantology, 2018; 16: 87–94.
[22]
Matsuura A, Aiba N, Yamamoto H, Takahashi M, Kido H, Suzuki T, et al. Stroking a Real Horse Versus Stroking a Toy Horse: Effects on the Frontopolar Area of the Human Brain. Anthrozoös. 2020; 33: 673–683.
[23]
Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang AG, Buchner A. G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods. 2007; 39: 175–191.
[24]
Fillmore MT, Rush CR, Kelly TH, Hays L. Triazolam impairs inhibitory control of behavior in humans. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 2001; 9: 363–371.
[25]
Wirt T, Hundsdörfer V, Schreiber A, Kesztyüs D, Steinacker JM. Associations between inhibitory control and body weight in German primary school children. Eating Behaviors. 2014; 15: 9–12.
[26]
Duan X, Shi J, Wu J, Mou Y, Cui H, Wang G. Electrophysiological correlates for response inhibition in intellectually gifted children: a Go/NoGo study. Neuroscience Letters. 2009; 457: 45–48.
[27]
Xu Y, Zhang W, Zhang K, Feng M, Duan T, Chen Y, et al. Basketball training frequency is associated with executive functions in boys aged 6 to 8 years. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2022; 16: 917385.
[28]
Cook R, Frederick EL. Incorporating Game in Hippotherapy A Companion Book to the Brown Pony Series. Createspace Independent Publishing Platform: USA. 2016.
[29]
Ulrich R, Prislan L, Miller J. A bimodal extension of the Eriksen flanker task. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics. 2021; 83: 790–799.
[30]
Shibuya-Tayoshi S, Sumitani S, Kikuchi K, Tanaka T, Tayoshi S, Ueno SI, et al. Activation of the prefrontal cortex during the Trail-Making Test detected with multichannel near-infrared spectroscopy. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences. 2007; 61: 616–621.
[31]
Song Q, Cheng X, Zheng R, Yang J, Wu H. Effects of different exercise intensities of race-walking on brain functional connectivity as assessed by functional near-infrared spectroscopy. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2022; 16: 1002793.
[32]
Anderson SW, Damasio H, Jones RD, Tranel D. Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance as a measure of frontal lobe damage. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 1991; 13: 909–922.
[33]
Li DY. Meta-analysis of the influence of exercise intervention based on medical images on the inhibitory control function of adolescents. Network Modeling and Analysis in Health Informatics and Bioinformatics. 2021; 10: 54.
[34]
Formenti D, Trecroci A, Duca M, Cavaggioni L, D’Angelo F, Passi A, et al. Differences in inhibitory control and motor fitness in children practicing open and closed skill sports. Scientific Reports. 2021; 11: 4033.
[35]
Di Russo F, Bultrini A, Brunelli S, Delussu AS, Polidori L, Taddei F, et al. Benefits of sports participation for executive function in disabled athletes. Journal of Neurotrauma. 2010; 27: 2309–2319.
[36]
Lin CHJ, Chiang MC, Knowlton BJ, Iacoboni M, Udompholkul P, Wu AD. Interleaved practice enhances skill learning and the functional connectivity of fronto-parietal networks. Human Brain Mapping. 2013; 34: 1542–1558.
[37]
Best JR. Effects of Physical Activity on Children’s Executive Function: Contributions of Experimental Research on Aerobic Exercise. Developmental Review. 2010; 30: 331–551.
[38]
Bass MM, Duchowny CA, Llabre MM. The effect of therapeutic horseback riding on social functioning in children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2009; 39: 1261–1267.
[39]
Ward SC, Whalon K, Rusnak K, Wendell K, Paschall N. The association between therapeutic horseback riding and the social communication and sensory reactions of children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2013; 43: 2190–2198.
[40]
Byun K, Hyodo K, Suwabe K, Ochi G, Sakairi Y, Kato M, et al. Positive effect of acute mild exercise on executive function via arousal-related prefrontal activations: an fNIRS study. NeuroImage. 2014; 98: 336–345.
[41]
Xuan B, Mackie MA, Spagna A, Wu T, Tian Y, Hof PR, et al. The activation of interactive attentional networks. NeuroImage. 2016; 129: 308–319.
[42]
Thiebaut de Schotten M, Dell’Acqua F, Forkel SJ, Simmons A, Vergani F, Murphy DGM, et al. A lateralized brain network for visuospatial attention. Nature Neuroscience. 2011; 14: 1245–1246.
[43]
Asanowicz D, Marzecová A, Jaśkowski P, Wolski P. Hemispheric asymmetry in the efficiency of attentional networks. Brain and Cognition. 2012; 79: 117–128.
[44]
Nee DE, Wager TD, Jonides J. Interference resolution: insights from a meta-analysis of neuroimaging tasks. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience. 2007; 7: 1–17.
[45]
Wang B, Guo W, Zhou C. Selective enhancement of attentional networks in college table tennis athletes: a preliminary investigation. PeerJ. 2016; 4: e2762.
[46]
Yu M, Xu S, Hu H, Li S, Yang G. Differences in right hemisphere fNIRS activation associated with executive network during performance of the lateralized attention network tast by elite, expert and novice ice hockey athletes. Behavioural Brain Research. 2023; 443: 114209.
[47]
Milham MP, Banich MT, Barad V. Competition for priority in processing increases prefrontal cortex’s involvement in top-down control: an event-related fMRI study of the stroop task. Cognitive Brain Research. 2003; 17: 212–222.
[48]
MacDonald AW, 3rd, Cohen JD, Stenger VA, Carter CS. Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. Science. 2000; 288: 1835–1838.
[49]
Vanderhasselt MA, De Raedt R, Baeken C, Leyman L, Clerinx P, D’haenen H. The influence of rTMS over the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex on top-down attentional processes. Brain Research. 2007; 1137: 111–116.

Publisher’s Note: IMR Press stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share
Back to top