IMR Press / JIN / Volume 23 / Issue 5 / DOI: 10.31083/j.jin2305088
Open Access Original Research
Event-related Potentials Indicate Target Processing in the Absence of Distractor Suppression during Rapid Serial Visual Presentation
Show Less
1 Department of Psychology, University of Zurich, 8050 Zurich, Switzerland
2 Department of Psychology, University of Galway, H91 TK33 Galway, Ireland
3 Department of Psychology, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität München, 80802 Munich, Germany
*Correspondence: p.sauseng@psychologie.uzh.ch (Paul Sauseng)
These authors contributed equally.
J. Integr. Neurosci. 2024, 23(5), 88; https://doi.org/10.31083/j.jin2305088 (registering DOI)
Submitted: 30 November 2023 | Revised: 15 March 2024 | Accepted: 21 March 2024 | Published: 25 April 2024
Copyright: © 2024 The Author(s). Published by IMR Press.
This is an open access article under the CC BY 4.0 license.
Abstract

Background: In our modern world we are exposed to a steady stream of information containing important as well as irrelevant information. Therefore, our brains have to constantly select relevant over distracting items and further process the selected information. Whereas there is good evidence that even in rapid serial streams of presented information relevant targets can be actively selected, it is less clear whether and how distracting information is de-selected and suppressed in such scenarios. Methods: To address this issue we recorded electroencephalographic activity during a rapid serial visual presentation paradigm in which healthy, young human volunteers had to encode visual targets into short-term memory while salient visual distractors and neutral filler items needed to be ignored. Event-related potentials were analyzed in 3D source space and compared between stimulus types. Results: A negative wave between around 170 and 230 ms after stimulus onset resembling the N2pc component was identified that dissociated between target stimuli and distractors as well as filler items. This wave appears to reflect target selection processes. However, there was no electrophysiological signature identified that would indicate an active distractor suppression mechanism. Conclusions: The obtained results suggest that unlike in situations where target stimuli and distractors are presented simultaneously, targets can be selected without the need for active suppression of distracting information in serial presentations with a clear and regular temporal structure. It is assumed that temporal expectation supports efficient target selection by the brain.

Keywords
attention
electroencephalography (EEG)
N2pc
distractor positivity
short-term memory
temporal expectation
1. Introduction

The human brain constantly has to select relevant over irrelevant information. For visual attention such selection processes are of uttermost importance [1, 2, 3], implemented via top-down control in visual search, for instance [4]. It has been suggested that this is achieved by applying so called activation maps [4]. However, frequently, salience of visual information determines which stimuli are selected for further processes [3]. And this selection does not necessarily have to be voluntarily. The dimension-weighting account proposes that feature maps can be influenced by previous experience and pre-attentive processes - amplifying and down-regulating the processing of different kind of information in parallel [5, 6]. This way not only selection of target stimuli but also suppression of distractors can be achieved [6, 7].

Suppression of salient distractors that co-occur with visual target stimuli seems effortful, requiring an active inhibition process that can be quantified using the so-called distractor positivity component PD in the event-related potential (ERP) acquired in electroencephalographic (EEG) recordings [8, 9]. This positive deflection in the ERP can usually be found over posterior recording sites contralateral to the visual hemifield to which distractors are presented around 200 ms after stimulus onset [8]. As a neural signature of target selection, however, a negative ERP wave strongest over posterior sites contralateral to target presentation and occurring shortly after the PD deflection (at around 250 ms after stimulus onset) has been characterized – the N2pc component [8, 10, 11, 12]. Doro et al. [13] could even show that a bilateral N2pc-like component (N2pcb) can be obtained when non-lateralized targets presented in visual midline are processed. While there is much evidence supporting the idea that the N2pc reflects target selection, it has also been discussed that it could potentially also reflect successful distractor suppression [14].

The N2pc and PD components as substrates of target selection and distractor suppression, respectively, have usually been obtained when targets and distractors were synchronously presented (typically with either only targets or only distractors lateralized, [8]). However, frequently in daily life there are situations where relevant and irrelevant information occur sequentially, and where the brain needs to rapidly decide which information to further process and which information to block out. And it was shown previously that sequential processing of relevant and irrelevant information can lead to changes in attentional functions (such as the attentional blink [15] or inter-trail priming effects on attentional priority [16]). When there are multiple sequential targets to be selected from a series of distractors/non-targets it is well established that targets elicit a strong evoked response (P300) after target onset as a signature of successful selection and further processing in working memory (e.g., [17, 18]). A similar ERP pattern can be observed in response to distractors—but only if they are falsely selected instead of a target [18]. A specific ERP response reflecting active inhibition of distractors in rapid serial presentation of relevant as well as irrelevant visual items, similar to the PD component that can be observed during simultaneous presentation of targets and distractors, has not been demonstrated so far, however. Moreover, as discussed above, it is not completely clear whether the N2pc reflects target selection or successful distractor suppression when targets and distractors are presented simultaneously [14]. By having a sequential presentation where targets are presented temporally independent from distractors, this ambiguity can be addressed.

Sequential presentation of targets and distractors implicate that in addition to features such as spatial location, color, shape, salience, etc., temporal information might be used in processing and dissociating targets from distractors. It has been shown that visuospatial and temporal attention can go hand in hand, and thus, support attentional processing of incoming sensory information [19]. Recently, it was reported that temporal predictability can even protect information stored in working memory from distraction [20, 21].

This is why we ran a delayed match-to-sample task in which in the encoding phase of each trial a series of rapidly highlighted spatial positions in a square matrix was presented to typically developed young human volunteers while EEG was recorded from the scalp. The rapid serial visual presentation mainly consisted of “filler items”, i.e., positions highlighted in gray, which the volunteers were supposed to ignore. Within each trial there were, however, five positions that were highlighted in green (“targets”) and three positions highlighted in red (“distractors”). Participants had to retain the target positions and later compare (match) them to a probe with five positions marked in green. The red “distractors” were highly salient [22, 23] but should be ignored by the volunteers. Importantly, the temporal structure of trials was always the same so that participants could potentially use temporal predictability of distractor occurrence as a mechanism to efficiently filter out distracting information.

We analyzed ERPs for the three stimulus categories (targets, distractors, fillers) in source space and hypothesized that a negative deflection around 250 ms after stimulus onset similar to the N2pc (or the non-lateralized N2pcb [13]) should be pronounced for targets compared to distractors and filler items. Encoding of targets into short-term memory in rapid serial presentations should, moreover, elicit a stronger P300-like ERP component in targets compared to distractors and filler items. As a signature of active distractor suppression we also expected a positive going wave around 200 ms after stimulus onset similar to the PD component for distractors compared to filler items and targets.

2. Materials and Methods
2.1 Participants

23 young healthy volunteers participated in the study at the LMU Munich, faculty 11 (where data were recorded; approval number: 18-2015-Sauseng-a), after giving written informed consent. Due to EEG recording malfunctions data from two participants could not be used for data analysis. The remaining sample of 21 volunteers had a mean age of 21.2 years (standard error of the mean (SEM) = 0.62) and consisted of 13 female and 8 male participants. They were all right-handed. A 12 plate Ishihara Test was used to confirm that no participant was color blind. The study was approved by the ethics review board at LMU Munich, faculty 11 (where data were recorded; approval number: 18-2015-Sauseng-a) and conducted according to the Declaration of Helsinki. All participants gave written informed consent and voluntarily took part in the experiment.

2.2 Experimental Paradigm

A visual delayed match-to-sample paradigm (Fig. 1) was run in which in each of 200 trials a rapid serial visual presentation was shown during an encoding phase. The monitor at which the paradigm was presented had a refresh rate of 60 Hz. At a rate of 6 Hz individual positions in a 5 × 5 square matrix (5.25° × 5.25° visual angle) were highlighted. 20 of these presentations were highlighted in grey. Participants were told that these were filler items and should not be attended. Within each rapid serial visual presentation stream five positions were highlighted in green (targets). Volunteers were instructed to retain these spatial target positions in memory and compare them to five positions shown in a retrieval probe after a 2000 ms retention period. In 50% of the trials the five positions in the retrieval probe matched the targets during the rapid serial visual presentation. Participants indicated by button press after each trial whether or not their memory representation matched the probe. Three frames in the serial presentation were highlighted in red. These were salient distractors and had to be ignored by the participants as well, similar to the filler items. Yet, due to their salient nature ignoring the red distractors should be more demanding. The colors for highlighting spatial positions (gray, green and red) were isoluminant. Targets and salient distractors did never overlap spatially, i.e., targets were not presented at any locations where a distractor would appear in a given trial, and vice versa.

Fig. 1.

Experimental Paradigm. In each trial of this delayed match-to-sample task a rapid serial visual presentation constituted the encoding phase. Spatial positions in a square matrix were sequentially highlighted at a rate of 6 Hz (stimulus onset asynchrony of roughly 167 ms). The majority of stimuli were gray filler items. Five of the frames (highlighted in green) were targets that had to be selected and encoded in short-term memory in order to compare their positions to a retrieval probe. Three stimulation frames in the rapid serial visual presentation stream were salient (red) distractors.

The temporal structure of the rapid serial visual presentation stream was identical in each trial. General presentation rate was 6 Hz. Targets were presented at a rate of 1.5 Hz. Distractor presentation rate was 0.75 Hz.

2.3 EEG Recordings

Scalp EEG was recorded from 60 channels placed according to the 10-10-system using Ag-AgCl ring electrodes and a BrainAmp 64 channel amplifier (BrainProducts®, Gilching, Germany). Additionally, signals from the left and right mastoids and horizontal and vertical electrooculogram (EOG) were recorded. Data were sampled at a rate of 1000 Hz. Impedances were kept below 10 kOhm during data recording.

2.4 Data Analysis

For each participant false alarm rate was subtracted from the hit rate and multiplied by the number of to-be retained items (five in this case) as an estimate of short-term memory capacity K [24]. If a participant performed perfectly on the task K would equal 5. If a participant performed on chance level their K value would equal 0 ((0.5 false alarm rate – 0.5 hit rate) × 5 = 0).

EEG analysis was carried out using BESA 7.1 software (BESA®, Gräfelfing, Germany). First, a 50 Hz notch filter was applied followed by a multiple source approach for EOG artifact correction [25] as implemented in BESA 7.1 whenever horizontal EOG signal was larger than 150 µV or vertical EOG was larger than 250 µV in amplitude. Using this EOG source based artifact correction [25] subsequently analyzed ERPs should not contain any effects resulting from eye movements. EEG artifacts were automatically marked and excluded from further analysis when after correction the signal at any EEG channel was exceeding 120 µV in amplitude or if it was constantly below 0.01 µV, as well as if the signal gradient exceeded 75 µV. Thereafter, EEG data were zero-phase shift filtered between 0.1 Hz (slope of 12 dB/oct) and 30 Hz (slope of 24 dB/oct). For each participant segments of –200 ms to 500 ms around stimulus onset were epoched for targets, distractors and filler items. These segments were then averaged into ERPs for each participant separately for the three stimulus categories. On average 921 (SEM = 35.5) segments were used per participant for the target ERP, 546 (SEM = 21.0) for distractors and 3449 (SEM = 132.7) segments for the filler ERPs. The large number of trials even in the distractor condition should have reduced the noise level to an extend that unequal trial numbers across conditions are considered as negligible factor.

To get rid of volume conduction effects that can lead to spurious results on scalp level, ERPs were analyzed in 3D source space next. Note that we were mainly interested in the temporal evolution of the ERP signal and not so much in spatial information obtained from the EEG. Therefore, the topographic source of any effects obtained will not be interpreted—the sole purpose of EEG source reconstruction in this study was to (i) limit volume conduction effects, (ii) reduce the number of measurements (60 EEG scalp electrode sites to a couple of dipole locations). Grand average ERPs were calculated (individual ERPs averaged across participants for each stimulus category separately). Next the target grand average ERP was submitted to standardized low-resolution electromagnetic tomography (sLORETA) [26] as implemented in BESA 7.1 software. BESA 7.1 identified two local sLORETA maxima. Thus, two dipoles were set into these local activity maxima. Orientation of the two dipoles was then fit to explain maximal spatial variance of the grand average ERP scalp topography. The resulting two-dipole montage explained a maximum of 97.1% of spatial variance in the target grand average ERP. The same procedure was applied to distractor and filler grand average ERPs. A two-dipole solution explained 89.7% of topographic variance in the distractor grand average scalp ERP. The filler ERP was best explained by a four-dipole solution. Still, only 64.3% of scalp topographic variance could be explained using this source montage.

The target source montage was then applied to target, distractor and filler ERPs for each participant separately. This way individual ERPs were obtained for each of the (two) dipole sources. Using the BESA statistics 2.1 package (BESA®, Gräfelfing, Germany), cluster-based permutation analysis of variance (ANOVA) tests [27] were run comparing target, distractor and filler ERPs obtained with the target source montage. In 1000 permutations a cluster alpha of 0.001 was used to identify temporal clusters between 0 and 500 ms after stimulus onset in which the three experimental conditions differed significantly. This was followed-up with cluster-based permutation post-hoc t-tests comparing the conditions in a pair-wise manner (i.e., target vs. distractor, target vs. filler, distractor vs. filler). The identical statistical analysis approach was then used on source ERPs obtained by applying the distractor (two-) dipole source montage and the filler (four-) dipole source montage to all three experimental conditions. This was done to make sure that also distractor and filler-related activities were optimally captured in at least one of the analyses. It was, however, not possible to directly compare data from the target montage, distractor montage and filler montage, since they comprised different locations and numbers of dipole sources.

As an exploratory analysis behavioral parameters (i.e., individual K values) were correlated with target and distractor source ERPs, again using a cluster-based permutation testing approach as implemented in BESA statistics 2.1.

For completeness, ERPs were also analyzed on scalp level. For each participant ERPs for targets, distractors and filler items were calculated. Similar to statistical analysis on source level we used BESA statistics 2.1 package to run cluster-based permutation ANOVA tests [27] comparing target, distractor and filler ERPs. In 1000 permutations a cluster alpha of 0.001 was used to identify clusters over time and electrode positions between 0 and 500 ms after stimulus onset in which the three experimental conditions differed significantly. This was followed-up with cluster-based permutation post-hoc t-tests comparing the conditions in a pair-wise manner (i.e., target vs. distractor, target vs. filler, distractor vs. filler).

The temporal sequence of target, distractor and filler presentation was identical across trials. This bears the problem that the second and third distractor followed target presentations (with always one filler in-between), whereas the first distractor was not preceded by a target. Hence, there is the possibility that the second and third distractors did not need to be actively suppressed as they exactly fell into potential attentional blinks [15] resulting from the preceding targets. To control for that, distractor-related ERPs for the first, second and third distractor presentation were compared on scalp level. Again, cluster-based permutation ANOVA tests [27] were run, this time comparing the three distractor positions.

3. Results

Average memory capacity K in the current task was 2.41 (SEM = 0.26) indicating that on average participants performed well within a margin clearly above chance level as well as below ceiling.

After applying the source montage derived from target ERPs to all three conditions, cluster-based permutation ANOVA tests over time identified only one single cluster with a significant difference between the three experimental conditions. This cluster was obtained for the more medial of the two dipole sources (see Fig. 2A) and stretched over the time interval between 172 to 223 ms after stimulus onset (maximal F-value = 72.6; all Fs >6.8, p < 0.001). During this temporal cluster, a strong negative deflection can be observed in response to target items, with clearly reduced amplitude in distractor and filler items. Cluster-based permutation post-hoc testing indicated that within this cluster there was a significant difference between targets and distractors (178 to 223 ms; p < 0.001) and between targets and filler items (173 to 221 ms; p < 0.001). Importantly, however, no significant difference was obtained between distractors and fillers.

Fig. 2.

ERP results on source level. When the source montage derived from target ERPs was applied to all three stimulus conditions (A) one cluster (at the dipole source marked in blue) stretching between 172 and 223 ms after stimulus onset dissociated between targets and distractors/fillers. Note, there was no significant difference between distractors and filler items. Largely identical results were obtained when a source montage based on distractor ERPs (B) or filler ERPs (C) was applied to all the conditions. There was always only one significant cluster identified dissociating between the three stimulus categories (driven by a larger negative wave for targets); and the cluster always stretched over an interval from roughly 170 to close to 230 ms. In none of these cases there was any significant difference between distractors and fillers. Colored dots (blue, red, pink or green) represent the locations of dipole sources (SDs). The dipole sources displaying significant differences between conditions (as indicated by pink shading in the amplitude and F-value graphs) are marked by a circle around the dot (i.e. in A the red dipole source would be SD-1, the blue dipole source is SD-2. The same accounts for B and C.). ERP, event-related potential; L, left; R, right; A, anterior; P, posterior.

Very similar results were found when the source montage obtained from distractors (see Fig. 2B) and that from filler items (see Fig. 2C) were applied to the ERP data. Using the distractor montage, again, only one significant cluster was observed at which the three experimental conditions differed significantly (173 to 223 ms; maximal F-value = 60.3; all Fs >5.1; p < 0.001); with no significant cluster-based permutation post-hoc tests on the p < 0.001 level between targets and distractors or between distractors and fillers but a significant difference between targets and filler items (173 to 221 ms). Similarly, one significant cluster on the main effect for stimulus condition was obtained after applying the filler item source montage to all stimulus categories (172 to 228 ms; maximal F-value = 61.2; p < 0.001) with post-hoc tests indicating that targets differed significantly (p < 0.001) from distractors (183 to 228 ms) and filler items (173 to 225 ms) but without any significant difference between distractors and fillers.

A comparison between targets, distractors and fillers was also run on EEG scalp level. Similar to source level there was one significant cluster stretching between 165 and 230 ms after stimulus onset dissociating between the conditions (maximal F-value = 37.8; all Fs >9.43, p < 0.001). The cluster extended over occipital, parietal and temporal recording sites (Fig. 3), with a stronger negative wave in response to targets compared to distractors and fillers peaking around 200 ms after stimulus onset. As obtained on source level, post-hoc tests on scalp level revealed a significant difference (p < 0.001) between targets and distractors as well as targets versus fillers, with no significant difference between distractors and fillers.

Fig. 3.

ERP results on scalp level. Comparing targets, distractors and filler items on scalp level revealed one significant (p < 0.001) spatio-temporal cluster covering occipital, parietal and temporal EEG recording sites (indicated with three asterisks each). The three conditions differed from each other in a time interval comparable to source level, again with targets eliciting a stronger negative wave than distractors and fillers, but distractors and fillers not significantly differing from each other. The lower panels in the figure depict scalp ERPs at recording site PO8 and cluster F-values. Red shading indicates the time interval of the significant effect. EEG, electroencephalography.

The temporal structure of trials led to the fact that the second and third distractors were preceded by targets, whereas this was not the case for the first distractor. If, therefore, attentional blinks following targets made it unnecessary to actively inhibit distractors, we should find a significant ERP difference between the first (that was not preceded by a target) versus the second and third distractor in each trial. Cluster-based permutation ANOVA comparing first, second and third distractors on the scalp level revealed a significant (p < 0.001) spatio-temporal cluster ranging from 160 to 500 ms after stimulus onset (with short interruptions between 204 and 247, as well as 279 to 302 ms; maximal F-value = 28.9; all Fs >7.9). Post-hoc comparisons indicate significant (p < 0.001) differences between first and second distractor, as well as second and third. However, there was no significant difference between first and third distractor.

Exploratory cluster-based permutation Pearson’s correlations between ERP amplitude in source space (using the target source montages for all ERPs) and individual K-values were run. The expectation was that the target-related negative wave should correlate negatively with short-term memory capacity (stronger negative wave associated with higher [positive] K-values), whereas this correlation should be in the opposite direction for distractor-related negative waves. However, no single significant cluster was obtained indicating no correlation between K-values and any of the ERP amplitudes (all |r| < 0.52; all p > 0.27).

4. Discussion

Similar as in experimental paradigms in which targets and distractors are presented simultaneously, we had expected that also during rapid serial visual presentation targets would elicit a stronger negative going wave around 200 ms after stimulus onset. Distractors had been expected to show a PD-like positive wave associated with active distractor suppression. The first hypothesis was confirmed, as we were able to observe a strong negative deflection associated with target processing in a time interval between about 170 to 230 ms after stimulus onset. This negative wave was neither observed in response to distractors nor to filler items. Contrary to our expectations, however, we were not able to identify any ERP pattern that was associated with active suppression of distractors. There was no positive deflection similar to a PD component previously reported as a signature of distractor suppression [8, 9]. Schankin and Schubö [14] speculated whether the N2pc could also reflect successful distractor suppression. Since in the current experiment targets and distractors were not presented simultaneously, and distractors did not elicit a strong negative going wave around 200 ms after stimulus onset, this indicates that it is more likely that the N2pc indeed reflects target selection rather than distractor suppression.

Our results suggest that during rapid serial visual presentation of targets and distractors the brain actively selects targets for further processing (e.g., storage into short-term memory) while distractors seem ignored by simply not selecting them. Since even before the start of each single trial, participants in the current study had already known exactly which items were targets (namely the green squares) they were able to tune their attentional filter towards the relevant features—making the selection process the relevant operation to solve the current task. In attentional blink paradigms [15] it is also argued that attentional filters are tuned towards identification of the first of two targets [28, 29]. Without any effort this target can then be identified within a series of similar distractors (e.g., within a stream of letters presented sequentially). Parallel presentation of targets and distractors simultaneously as in paradigms in which PD ERP components have been reported [8, 9], far more likely leads to direct competition between stimuli. This will then require additional active inhibition processes. The Boost-and-Bounce Theory of Attention [30] suggests that the exact temporal structure of target and distractor presentation influences whether targets can be further processed or not. Thereby, it should be particularly difficult to actively suppress a distractor; whereas distractors in return would lead to increased interference with previous targets. Dell’Acqua and colleagues [31] investigated whether this distractor driven suppression of attention might be reflected by a frontal negative ERP wave and did only find very limited evidence for that. In the present study, we do not find any distractor-related negative wave. This might, however, have to do with the fact that in the current experimental design salient distractors did not directly follow presentation of targets.

In a recent study by Forschack et al. [32] lateralized targets and distractors were presented simultaneously; but stimulus locations were tagged at different frequencies so that steady-state evoked potentials could be obtained. Whereas steady-state evoked potential amplitude increase was observed at target locations, there was no amplitude attenuation at distractor locations; i.e., similar to our current main findings there was no evidence for active distractor suppression. In Forschack et al.’s study [32] only one target location and one distractor location was shown. Therefore, it was argued that most likely in small set-size search display, initially, targets as well as distractors capture attention; and in a second stage targets are selected by amplification of their neural response [32].

While set size was larger in the current study, only one feature, i.e., color, dissociated targets from distractors and filler items. However, the reason why in the current design no active distractor suppression mechanism was obtained might also have to do with the temporal structure of trials. The sequence of stimuli was identical across trials with a fixed frequency at which targets, distractors and filler items were presented. Therefore, temporal expectations may have supported efficient selection and processing of targets [19] making it unnecessary to actively suppress distractors. And indeed it is known from working memory paradigms that temporal predictivity can support shielding memory content from distraction [20, 21]. A similar beneficial effect of temporal predictivity seems very plausible in the current task where participants acquired information about the exact timing of distractor appearance.

In the current study participants were not only required to select targets among distractors and fillers, but they were also asked to store targets in short-term memory and compare them to a probe after a short delay interval. In rapid serial visual presentation studies encoding of targets has frequently been associated with a P300-like positive wave in ERPs (e.g., [17, 18]). We did not find such deflection dissociating between targets and distractors or filler items in our study. Moreover, the ERP effects in the current experiment did not correlate with short-term memory task performance. This supports the notion that the negative N2pc-like wave here does indeed merely reflect selection of targets without encoding into short-term memory. Moreover, a lateralized slow negative wave, the contralateral delay activity (CDA), is more frequently been associated with short-term memory capacity rather than earlier ERP components [33, 34]. The rapid serial presentation in the current study, however, might not be optimal for detecting such slow negative waves. Encoding and maintenance of targets in rapid serial visual presentation might rely on more distributed processes that cannot be easily captured by activity from a single cortical source. Glennon and co-workers [28], for instance, were able to demonstrate that in an attentional blink paradigm encoding and maintenance of targets depended on coherent, rhythmical activity at theta frequency within a left and medial to right temporo-parietal cortical network. Since in the current study dipole analysis was purely data driven it was not possible to investigate for instance fronto-parietal interactions. It should also be noted that 3D source analysis here was merely used for reducing volume conduction effects, reducing number of sites and increasing the signal to noise ratio. The purpose of source analysis in the current study never was to investigate where in the brain targets are selected and processed; the aim was to investigate the temporal evolution of the process and to investigate if and when distractors get dissociated from targets; 3D source analysis was merely applied as a method by which information from all scalp recording channels was used for obtaining temporal neural information. This is also why we do not interpret the location of identified dipole sources in the brain.

Comparison between targets, distractors and filler items was also run on scalp level. These results (Fig. 3) resemble those from the 3D source analysis very well. The spatial extend of the effect is large, covering all occipital, parietal and temporal scalp recording sites. As in the source analysis there was an N2pcb-like negative wave that dissociated targets from distractors and fillers in the absence of any significant difference between distractors and filler items. Same as in source space, no additional potential distractor suppression-related effect was found on scalp level.

In each trial the second and third distractor in the sequence were always preceded by presentation of a target by 334 ms. Thus, these distractors fell into the time window where after a target presentation the attentional blink could be expected [15]. This could potentially explain why no active distractor suppression was necessary in the current task. However, a comparison between ERPs to the first, second and third distractor presentation separately only indicates a deviation of the second distractor throughout most of the analysis time window (speaking for a rather unspecific effect which might be caused by a baseline shift). There was no significant difference between the first and the third distractor. This makes it rather unlikely that the attentional blink was responsible for distractor suppression in the current task.

Another potential explanation for the lack of any distractor suppression-related ERP signature in the current data set could be that distractors might not have been salient enough. Color, however, is a feature of great importance when it comes to salience of stimuli, particularly distractors [22, 23, 35]. Targets and distractors colored in red (as compared to green) among grey stimuli captured attention most strongly in a visual search task despite matched luminance—an effect also reflected by N2pc and PD components, respectively [22, 23]. Moreover, in a previous study we were able to show that the attentional blink was particularly pronounced if the first target within a rapid serial visual presentation was colored in red with the second target being shown in grey [28]. Considering reports demonstrating that red distractors even lead to a stronger PD component than distractors presented at another color [22] it is even more surprising that we did not find any PD (neither for distractors nor for fillers) at all. In addition to red color as a distractor salience defining feature, it had been shown previously that particularly the relative salience compared to neutral stimuli (in our case fillers) is what decides whether distractors need to be actively suppressed or not [36, 37]. This, however, accounts for paradigms with simultaneous presentation of targets and distractors where the distractor mostly pops out as a singleton. Therefore, in future work, it should be addressed whether a reduced number of distractors per trial or a higher salience of distractors than that of targets could also lead to neural signatures of distractor suppression in serial presentations, such as in the current study. While there is evidence from literature that the distractors in the current study should be highly salient and therefore should be more likely to capture attention than, e.g., the grey filler items, a clear limitation of this study is a lack of behavioral evidence for this assumption. Therefore, in future studies the paradigm could be slightly changed, so that in part of the trials the red distractors are left out. If indeed the distractors are more distracting due to higher salience than the fillers, one would expect attenuated task performance in trials with red distractors present. Likewise, if the experiment was changed so that within trials distractor and target locations can overlap, it would be potentially possible to observe detrimental effects on target encoding if the target was presented at a location preceded or followed by a distractor. However, our current ERP findings suggest that distractors are not really actively suppressed in this kind of experimental paradigm. Instead, targets are actively enhanced and selected (reflected by increased N2pc amplitudes). This might make it very difficult to find behavioral correlates for distraction in the current task, at all, even if salience of distractors is further increased.

5. Conclusions

Whereas in experiments where targets and distractors are presented simultaneously separate ERP signatures of target selection and distractor suppression have been reported, here we do not find any ERP phenomenon associated with active distractor suppression. A negative wave between around 170 and 230 ms after stimulus onset as a clear signature for target selection was obtained however. Most likely the fast serial presentation of targets and distractors and temporal predictability allowed the brain to efficiently tune attention filters making it unnecessary to actively suppress distractors.

Abbreviations

EEG, electroencephalogram; EOG, electrooculogram; ERP, event-related potential.

Availability of Data and Materials

EEG data and behavioural data are fully available at Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/6te9a/?view_only=43deb01766c84f83a6f582174c6d15ab).

Author Contributions

PS and MG conceptualized and implemented the study. MG recorded data. CP and PS analyzed data. All authors contributed to editorial changes in the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript. All authors have participated sufficiently in the work and agreed to be accountable for all aspects of the work.

Ethics Approval and Consent to Participate

The study was approved by the ethics review board at LMU Munich, faculty 11 (where data were recorded; approval number: 18-2015-Sauseng-a) and conducted according to the Declaration of Helsinki. All participants gave written informed consent and voluntarily took part in the experiment.

Acknowledgment

Not applicable.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References
[1]
Kastner S, Pinsk MA. Visual attention as a multilevel selection process. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience. 2004; 4: 483–500.
[2]
Kastner S, Ungerleider LG. Mechanisms of visual attention in the human cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2000; 23: 315–341.
[3]
Theeuwes J. Goal-driven, stimulus-driven, and history-driven selection. Current Opinion in Psychology. 2019; 29: 97–101.
[4]
Wolfe JM, Gray W. Guided search 4.0. Integrated Models of Cognitive Systems. 2007; 99–119.
[5]
Liesefeld HR, Liesefeld AM, Pollmann S, Müller HJ. Biasing Allocations of Attention via Selective Weighting of Saliency Signals: Behavioral and Neuroimaging Evidence for the Dimension-Weighting Account. Current Topics in Behavioral Neurosciences. 2019; 41: 87–113.
[6]
Liesefeld HR, Müller HJ. Distractor handling via dimension weighting. Current Opinion in Psychology. 2019; 29: 160–167.
[7]
Feldmann-Wüstefeld T, Weinberger M, Awh E. Spatially Guided Distractor Suppression during Visual Search. The Journal of Neuroscience: the Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2021; 41: 3180–3191.
[8]
Gaspelin N, Luck SJ. Combined Electrophysiological and Behavioral Evidence for the Suppression of Salient Distractors. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2018; 30: 1265–1280.
[9]
Gaspelin N, Luck SJ. The Role of Inhibition in Avoiding Distraction by Salient Stimuli. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2018; 22: 79–92.
[10]
Kiss M, Van Velzen J, Eimer M. The N2pc component and its links to attention shifts and spatially selective visual processing. Psychophysiology. 2008; 45: 240–249.
[11]
Seiss E, Kiss M, Eimer M. Does focused endogenous attention prevent attentional capture in pop-out visual search? Psychophysiology. 2009; 46: 703–717.
[12]
Schubö A, Schröger E, Meinecke C, Müller HJ. Attentional resources and pop-out detection in search displays. Neuroreport. 2007; 18: 1589–1593.
[13]
Doro M, Bellini F, Brigadoi S, Eimer M, Dell’Acqua R. A bilateral N2pc (N2pcb) component is elicited by search targets displayed on the vertical midline. Psychophysiology. 2020; 57: e13512.
[14]
Schankin A, Schubö A. Contextual cueing effects despite spatially cued target locations. Psychophysiology. 2010; 47: 717–727.
[15]
Shapiro KL, Raymond JE, Arnell KM. The attentional blink. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 1997; 1: 291–296.
[16]
Wirth BE, Ramgir A, Lamy D. Feature intertrial priming biases attentional priority: Evidence from the capture-probe paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 2023; 49: 1145–1157.
[17]
Vogel EK, Luck SJ, Shapiro KL. Electrophysiological evidence for a postperceptual locus of suppression during the attentional blink. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 1998; 24: 1656–1674.
[18]
Bourassa MÈ, Vachon F, Brisson B. Failure of temporal selectivity: Electrophysiological evidence for (mis)selection of distractors during the attentional blink. Psychophysiology. 2015; 52: 933–941.
[19]
Peylo C, Romberg-Taylor C, Behnke L, Sauseng P. Dynamic alpha power modulations and slow negative potentials track natural shifts of spatio-temporal attention. Psychophysiology. 2023; e14498.
[20]
Gresch D, Boettcher SEP, van Ede F, Nobre AC. Shielding working-memory representations from temporally predictable external interference. Cognition. 2021; 217: 104915.
[21]
Gresch D, Boettcher SEP, Nobre AC, van Ede F. Consequences of predictable temporal structure in multi-task situations. Cognition. 2022; 225: 105156.
[22]
Fortier-Gauthier U, Dell’acqua R, Jolicœur P. The “red-alert” effect in visual search: evidence from human electrophysiology. Psychophysiology. 2013; 50: 671–679.
[23]
Pomerleau VJ, Fortier-Gauthier U, Corriveau I, Dell’Acqua R, Jolicœur P. Colour-specific differences in attentional deployment for equiluminant pop-out colours: evidence from lateralised potentials. International Journal of Psychophysiology: Official Journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology. 2014; 91: 194–205.
[24]
Cowan N. The magical number 4 in short-term memory: a reconsideration of mental storage capacity. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 2001; 24: 87–114; discussion 114–185.
[25]
Berg P, Scherg M. A multiple source approach to the correction of eye artifacts. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. 1994; 90: 229–241.
[26]
Pascual-Marqui RD. Standardized low-resolution brain electromagnetic tomography (sLORETA): technical details. Methods and Findings in Experimental and Clinical Pharmacology. 2002; 24: 5–12.
[27]
Maris E, Oostenveld R. Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-data. Journal of Neuroscience Methods. 2007; 164: 177–190.
[28]
Glennon M, Keane MA, Elliott MA, Sauseng P. Distributed Cortical Phase Synchronization in the EEG Reveals Parallel Attention and Working Memory Processes Involved in the Attentional Blink. Cerebral Cortex (New York, N.Y.: 1991). 2016; 26: 2035–2045.
[29]
Di Lollo V, Kawahara JI, Shahab Ghorashi SM, Enns JT. The attentional blink: resource depletion or temporary loss of control? Psychological Research. 2005; 69: 191–200.
[30]
Olivers CNL, Meeter M. A boost and bounce theory of temporal attention. Psychological Review. 2008; 115: 836–863.
[31]
Dell’Acqua R, Doro M, Dux PE, Losier T, Jolicœur P. Enhanced frontal activation underlies sparing from the attentional blink: Evidence from human electrophysiology. Psychophysiology. 2016; 53: 623–633.
[32]
Forschack N, Gundlach C, Hillyard S, Müller MM. Electrophysiological evidence for target facilitation without distractor suppression in two-stimulus search displays. Cerebral Cortex (New York, N.Y.: 1991). 2022; 32: 3816–3828.
[33]
Vogel EK, McCollough AW, Machizawa MG. Neural measures reveal individual differences in controlling access to working memory. Nature. 2005; 438: 500–503.
[34]
Vogel EK, Machizawa MG. Neural activity predicts individual differences in visual working memory capacity. Nature. 2004; 428: 748–751.
[35]
Vierck E, Miller J. Distraction by color and its electrophysiological correlates. Psychophysiology. 2009; 46: 593–606.
[36]
Gaspar JM, Christie GJ, Prime DJ, Jolicœur P, McDonald JJ. Inability to suppress salient distractors predicts low visual working memory capacity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2016; 113: 3693–3698.
[37]
McDonald JJ, Gaspar JM, Lagroix HEP, Jolicœur P. Difficulty suppressing visual distraction while dual tasking. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2023; 30: 224–234.

Publisher’s Note: IMR Press stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share
Back to top